Skip to main content

Dynamics of the social construction of knowledge: an empirical study of Zhihu in China

Abstract

This study examined how the knowledge market promotes knowledge construction on question-and-answer (Q&A) websites. Data were collected from Zhihu, one of the largest Q&A sites in China. Hierarchical linear modeling was employed to estimate the dynamics of information accumulation, that is, the provision of informative content as factual construction. By employing information accumulation as the objective measure of knowledge construction, we determined that online knowledge construction was facilitated by a competitive marketplace of ideas. In addition, participation, temporal, and discourse features affected the dynamics of information accumulation. In specific, active users contributed significantly less to information accumulation than did ordinary users. Information accumulation shows a naturally decaying process represented as a function of answer order. The time interval between answers at the two preceding time points reduced the informativeness of answers at the subsequent time point. Answers with a higher readability score reduced the informativeness of subsequent answers. The results indicate that knowledge construction on Q&A sites unfolds as a process of mass collaboration among users.

1 Introduction

The structure of material commodity markets affects production. In contrast to competitive markets, monopolistic markets can determine the elasticity of demand and the price of a product [1]. In the marketplace of ideas, knowledge production is affected by the structure of an idea market. Whether the knowledge production process is more efficient in a competitive or monopolistic marketplace of ideas remains unclear. In a competitive marketplace of ideas, knowledge production is a social process in which the contributions of many participants are assembled through negotiation [2]. In a monopolistic marketplace of ideas, a few elite users dominate knowledge construction. A very small proportion of elite users, rather than a large crowd of average users [3], make substantial contributions to the advancement of knowledge.

Question-and-answer (Q&A) websites function as an online marketplace of ideas in which knowledge is constructed [4]. In these highly structured but open environments, freely editable content enhances the plurality of viewpoints in the knowledge construction process [5]. As a Q&A site, Zhihu.com is an example of a large-scale platform for knowledge construction. Zhihu is designed to allow people to ask and answer questions on a broad range of topics. Similar to Quora and StackOverflow, questions on Zhihu are resolved by user-generated answers. The question–answer threads on Zhihu are well-defined and easily identifiable artifacts [6]. A question–answer thread is a hierarchically organized collection of messages, with an initial answer to the original question and with subsequent messages written as answers to earlier messages [6] (please see the Supplementary Information for a sample page of Zhihu). Users can also post comments and reply to one another under each answer. This study used Zhihu to examine how the marketplace of ideas affects the dynamics of online knowledge construction. In particular, this study investigated whether online knowledge construction is better facilitated in a competitive marketplace of ideas or a monopolistic marketplace of ideas.

2 Concepts, theoretical framework, and hypotheses

2.1 Evaluating knowledge construction

Information accumulation is a key criterion of the success of the knowledge construction process on Q&A sites [7]. Knowledge construction on Q&A sites creates new ideas, explanations, and theories that help the members of a community understand the world [810]. According to Latour and Woolgar [11], information is accumulated in the knowledge construction process through the negotiation of new content among knowledge contributors. On Q&A sites, the provision of informative content follows an ordered progression of factual construction over time [5, 12].

However, information accumulation has been widely ignored in evaluations of knowledge construction on Q&A sites, especially Chinese Q&A sites. Studies have examined the information listed on Q&A sites by employing measures such as accuracy; completeness; article length; information richness; and the number of references, headings, and functional links [12], but few of them have employed the criterion of information accumulation. Therefore, the first objective of this study was to use information accumulation as an objective measure to examine the process of online knowledge construction in the context of Chinese Q&A sites, taking Zhihu as a case study.

2.2 Marketplace of ideas

The marketplace of ideas, a concept underlying the notion of freedom of expression, is based on an analogy with the economic concept of a free market. With regard to Q&A sites, the marketplace of ideas refers to the act of discussion among distributed contributors. Q&A sites have become an ideal manifestation of the marketplace of ideas. Q&A sites embrace the tenets of marketplace theory, namely (1) that ideas should be in competition; (2) that the public at large is the best provider of ideas; and (3) that exposure to contradictory points of view has value in the search for truth [13]. On Q&A sites, users compete with one another to dominate the discussion, demonstrating the competition of ideas on the open market, which is the key component of marketplace theory. Q&A sites also reduce barriers to entry in the marketplace of ideas because all opinions can be freely expressed, with the open exchange of ideas. Furthermore, an essential function of Q&A sites is the ability of users to read and reply multiple answers to a question and be exposed to conflicting points of view; this can promote the diversity of opinions. These characteristics make Q&A sites ideal online communities to which marketplace theory can be applied to study knowledge construction [13].

The second objective of this study was to investigate the contribution of the marketplace of ideas to information accumulation on Q&A sites. Q&A sites can be generally classified as a competitive marketplace of ideas or a monopolistic marketplace of ideas [14]. In a competitive marketplace of ideas, the sharing of contributor viewpoints can be understood as an exercise undertaken to resolve differences of opinion [5]. Answers on Q&A sites reveal competition among ideas. In addition, in a more competitive marketplace of ideas, free expression is self-correcting [15]. A consensus emerges from competition among ideas in a free, transparent public discourse, and ideas and ideologies are winnowed on the basis of their superiority or inferiority and their acceptance by the user population.

By contrast, in a monopolistic marketplace of ideas, the flow of information is controlled. The dominant viewpoints monopolizes the marketplace and drives the other information out, thus excluding rival opinions [16]. In a Q&A site that operates as a monopolistic marketplace of ideas, the critical thinking of knowledge contributors is suppressed. Therefore, in a monopolistic marketplace of ideas, less-diverse viewpoints are disseminated to the public, thus generating less new knowledge [17]. Therefore, we proposed the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 1

A more competitive marketplace of ideas contributes more to information accumulation than a more monopolistic marketplace of ideas does.

2.3 Participation features

On the basis of the study by Latour and Woolgar [11] and other studies on the collaborative construction of knowledge [2, 18], the present study further included participation features, temporal features, and discourse features as explanatory variables to explain the knowledge construction process.

Q&A sites promote social participation, which is pivotal to facilitating knowledge contributions because it promotes the development of social cohesion and a sense of belonging [810]. According to Gunawardena et al. [2], participation features determine the key stages of knowledge construction, which are the discovery and exploration of dissonance or inconsistency among ideas, concepts, or statements; the negotiation of meaning and co-construction of knowledge; and the testing and modification of the proposed synthesis or co-construction process. On Q&A sites, participation features may affect users’ motivation to contribute knowledge and the manner in which they share their existing knowledge.

2.3.1 Presence of active users

Participation feature in current study refers to the presence of active users. In online knowledge construction, most active users make crucial contributions to information accumulation. A widely observed phenomenon in online communication is the “90–9–1 rule.” This refers to the fact that in most online communities, 90% of users never contribute but only consume content, 9% of users have a little contribution, and 1% of users account for almost all of the contributions [1921]. Studies reported the presence of uneven contributions in online collaborative settings [22, 23]. For example, Serrano et al. [24] proposed a ratio metric to calculate the inequality of participation in Wikipedia: the quotient between the number of contributions from the top 10% contributors and the number of contributions from the other 90% users. They observe a power-law in the distribution of contributions. Movshovitz-Attias et al. highlighted the importance of the contribution of active users to the answers posted on StackOverflow [25]. Users with high numbers of contributions were usually the first responders to questions. If the questions remain unanswered, they are then answered by users with fewer number of contributions. Accordingly, we posited the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 2

Unlike ordinary users, active users promote information accumulation.

2.4 Temporal patterns of knowledge construction on Q&A sites

Temporal features refer to the time interval between answer posts and the order of answers. Temporal features result in the construction of knowledge in a hidden and unintentional manner [26, 27]. First, online knowledge creation is not a linear function of time. A marked reduction in the interval between answer posts on a site (i.e., a spike in answers posted in a given period) can serve as a proxy for a peak in online collaboration [28]. Q&A sites are web environments in which content is stored and managed collaboratively. They are also platforms on which users can showcase their knowledge and expertise to peers or potential recruiters [29]. Surges in posting behavior on Q&A sites can promote emergent collaboration [30] in terms of improvisation [31], heedful interrelating [32], privileging expertise [33], and knowledge shaping [34]. Users may provide more responses in a short period of time under external or internal triggers [30], such as a controversial topic, the presentation of biased information, or some form of vandalism (i.e., editing deliberately intended to defeat or obstruct other contributions). In this regard, a short time interval between answer posts indicates the burstiness of knowledge construction. Burstiness in this context is characterized by users collaboratively engaging in the knowledge construction process in a short period of time, during which knowledge is accumulated. Anderson et al. [35] observed that the majority of answers on Q&A sites are posted within a day after the question is asked. In addition, the longer a question remains unanswered, the lower the likelihood is that a satisfactory answer will be provided. Therefore, we postulated the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 3

The time interval between answer posts affects the dynamics of knowledge construction. The shorter the time interval between answer posts, the higher the level of information accumulation.

Second, information accumulation is a naturally decaying process. The amount of new information decreases along with the increase of the order of answers, because multiple users offer the same information or refer to the same source for answering a single question. The first answers tend to provide novel information, whereas answers provided at the end of the thread tend to be less informative. Anderson et al. [35] reported that on StackOverflow, the most informative answers usually appear earlier in the sequence of responses to a question. Therefore, information accumulation decreases with the order of answers because of the repeated referral to similar information. We presented the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 4

Information accumulation is a naturally decaying process represented as a function of answer order.

2.5 Discourse features

Discourse features refer to sentiment and readability. As noted by Latour and Woolgar [11], in online knowledge construction, the discourse is subjected to meaning negotiation. Information that conveys sentiment [36] and is easy to understand is more likely to stimulate online engagement.

2.5.1 Sentiment

In this study, we employed sentiment and readability as discourse factors. Sentiment can affect users’ knowledge contributions to a platform. Q&A sites facilitate numerous types of interactions. On these online collaboration platforms, members may be driven to answer questions for conveying their sentiment appropriately through text [37]. From the perspective of interpersonal communication, individuals’ sentiment toward a topic represents the atmosphere of the discussion on that topic. Expressions of gratitude and reciprocity are essential for developing trust and empathy among users, which increases their willingness to collaborate and the acquisition of answers on Q&A sites [38]. A study by Jiao et al. determined that replies that express positive sentiment (e.g., “that’s a great summary”) can lead to further communication [39]. By contrast, replies that convey negative sentiment (e.g., “I am so confused” and “Shut up”) are more likely to end a conversation [40].

Sentiment also indicates knowledge contributors’ interest in a topic. Positive sentiment of a topic on Q&A sites correspond to greater interest in that topic [41, 42]. If a group of people have positive sentiment for a topic, they are more likely to focus on that topic and contribute to the platform [43]. Accordingly, we advanced the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 5

The positive sentiment of an online discussion leads to the accumulation of information, whereas the negative sentiment of an online discussion reduces the accumulation of information.

2.5.2 Readability

Readability refers to the textual comprehensibility of a discourse [44]. On Q&A sites, readability represents the ease with which a piece of text can be understood [45]. Journal articles written for expert audiences and answers on Q&A sites written for nonexpert readers differ substantially in their readability [46]. Knowledge constructed by expert users with extensive domain-specific knowledge is often difficult to understand because such users assume that their readers are fellow experts who can easily comprehend abstruse scientific text [47]. Online knowledge construction on Q&A sites entails the reformulation of professional content for the non-specialist public through linguistic means, which supports the popularization of this content on the sites [48]. Knowledge constructed for general users who have little or no prior knowledge of a given topic or field must be easily understandable. Higher readability can increase the probability that the text conveys the correct information with minimal reading effort [45].

The readability of text on Q&A sites may affect the dynamics of knowledge construction on such sites [45]. The use of jargon-free language may increase the accessibility and acceptability of scientific knowledge to the non-specialist public. Readable language improves the reception of a post and attracts a greater number of general users, encouraging them to join the discussion. Therefore, we proposed the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 6

An online discussion with high readability facilitates the accumulation of information.

On the basis of the findings of Harper et al. [49] and Kim et al. [50], we classified questions posted on Q&A sites into three types: opinion based, experience based, and fact based. The question type represents the style of response required for the question. Opinion-based questions require others’ thoughts on a topic of general interest; these questions do not have a correct answer. Experience-based questions require that respondent’s share their experiences. Fact-based questions require the provision of objective information.

In addition, we employed the number of answers to each question as an additional control variable. The number of answers indicates the popularity of a question. We controlled for the number of answers to examine the effects of the marketplace type (i.e., competitive or monopolistic) on information accumulation.

3 Methods

3.1 Data collection and questions filtering

Data for this study were collected from Zhihu, a widely used Q&A site in China. Zhihu is an example of a large-scale, volunteer-led collaborative project to construct knowledge [15]. Zhihu was launched in 2011, and by 2019, the number of registered users had exceeded 220 million.Footnote 1

Large-scale datasets contain a high number of observations, which makes rejecting the null hypothesis at the selected alpha level considerably easier (\(p < 0.05\) in this study)[51]. Therefore, we randomly selected 10,000 questions and their answers from a list of retrieved questions (\(N = 1\text{,}600\text{,}000\)) from Zhihu. Figure 1 illustrates the data collection process. We filtered the questions using the following criteria. First, only questions that had more than 10 answers were retained. Second, only questions that were published between December 20, 2010 (Zhihu’s launch date), and September 28, 2019, were included. Third, only questions that were open to answers for more than 30 days were retained. The final sample comprised 87,912 answer records nested within 1832 questions.

Figure 1
figure 1

Data collection and filtering

3.2 Measurements

Information accumulation was measured at the answer level. For each question thread, we determined the dynamics of information accumulation by employing the computational approach proposed by Qi, Zhang, and Manning [52]. For a given question Q, H represents its historical answer sequence, which contains k answers before \(A_{i}\), where \(A_{i}\) is the ith answer of Q, as expressed by the following equation (Fig. 2):

$$ H= \{ A_{1}, A_{2}, A_{3},\ldots , A_{k} \}. $$
(1)
Figure 2
figure 2

Schematic of information accumulation measurement

The information accumulation of answer \(A_{i}\) represents the amount of new information the ith answer holds relative to the content of previous replies. The assumption is that the more the amount of new information \(A_{i}\) holds, the higher the likelihood is that it would include words not used in preceding answers. To quantify the information accumulation of \(A_{i}\), we first estimated the pairwise lexical similarity between \(A_{i}\) and all other preceding answers \(A_{k}\) in H (\(1 < k < i < n\), where n is the total number of answers). For example, for a question with four answers, the similarity of the fourth answer to the preceding three answers (i.e., the similarity between the fourth and third, fourth and second, and fourth and first answers) was estimated.

The Doc2vec algorithm [53] was employed to estimate the similarity between \(A_{i}\) and all preceding answers listed in a historical answer sequence H. An extension of Word2vec, which extends the learning of word embeddings from words to word sequences, Doc2vec converts a document to a distribution over words, which can be used in pairwise comparisons of the document similarity. The cosine similarity score ranges from −1 to 1. Because negative values of the similarity score are meaningless, we drop the observations with negative similarity scores from the analysis and constrained the similarity score from 0 to 1[54]. A similarity score of 0 indicates the absence of similarity between \(A_{i}\) and \(A_{k}\). A similarity score of 1 indicates that \(A_{i}\) is a replicate of the preceding answer \(A_{k}\). Appendix provides further validation of information accumulation (see Additional file 1).

The estimated similarity scores were used to calculate the information accumulation of the ith answer by using the following formula (2):

$$ IA_{\mathrm{ans}} ( A_{i} ) =1- \max_{1< k< i} \text{Similarity} ( A_{i}, A_{k} ). $$
(2)

3.2.1 Measurement validation

Manual coding was performed to validate the computational measurement of information accumulation. Two coders were recruited to rate the information accumulation of answers to each of 400 randomly selected questions. They conducted a pairwise comparison of \(A_{i}\) and each preceding answer in the sequence by answering the question “Does the given answer (i.e., \(A_{i}\)) provide new information?” with the possible responses of 1 (yes) and 0 (no) on a dichotomous scale. If any rating for a given answer \(A_{i}\) was 0, the information accumulation of \(A_{i}\) was given a final score of 0. This meant that \(A_{i}\) failed to provide new information. Intercoder reliability was adequate, as indicated by Krippendorff’s alpha of 0.82.

The information accumulation of all 400 questions was estimated using our computational algorithms. The estimated score was classified as a binary variable, with scores greater than 0.5 recoded as 1 and scores lower than 0.5 recoded as 0. The correlation between the human-coded results and the computationally estimated results of the information accumulation of the 400 questions was 0.68, which demonstrated that our computational algorithm yielded results of information accumulation that were consistent with those of human assessments (please see Additional file 1 for further validation of the measurement).

3.2.2 Marketplace of ideas

The marketplace of ideas was assessed at the answer level. For each answer (\(A_{i}\)) nested within a question, we obtained a distribution of the answer length (the number of characters in each answer) and the number of comments left on each preceding answer.

The Gini coefficients for the answer length and the number of comments were used to assess the marketplace of ideas of \(A_{i}\) before answer \(A_{i}\) was provided. The Gini coefficient, an entropy measure of the equality of distribution, is a measure of statistical dispersion and is representative of inequality. Therefore, a low Gini coefficient indicates a competitive marketplace of ideas, which lacks dominant viewpoints, whereas a high Gini coefficient indicates a monopolistic marketplace of ideas for given questions.

The Gini coefficient of the answer length indicates the equality of the distribution of the knowledge provided by contributors in the construction stage. The distribution of the number of comments for answers indicates the preferences of other users. The Gini coefficient of the number of comments indicates equality in the distribution of users’ feedback. A Gini coefficient of 0 for the number of comments indicates an equal distribution; that is, all answers received the same number of comments. A Gini coefficient of 1 represents maximum inequality; that is, all comments were left under one answer, and the other answers received no comments.

3.2.3 Presence of active users

To determine the presence of active users, we calculated the answering frequency of all users on Zhihu. On the basis of the 90–9–1 rule, we defined highly active, active, and ordinary users as those whose answering frequencies ranked in the top 1%, top 2%–10%, and bottom 90%, respectively. Anonymous users, who have private accounts and cannot be identified in the platform, were classified in a separate category. The users on the platform are fully anonymous. We cannot tell if two messages in the same discussion by anonymous users are by the same person.

3.2.4 Sentiment

Sentiment was calculated as the intensity scores of neutral, positive, and negative sentiment for a given answer. We used Senta, an open-source sentiment classification system established by Baidu for Chinese text sentiment analysis. We employed the BiLSTM (i.e., Bidirectional Long Short Term Memory) model, which is a recurrent neural network model that accepts Chinese corpus as input and outputs sentiment polarity [55, 56]. A score of 0 represents negative sentiment, and a score of 1 represents positive sentiment.

3.2.5 Readability

Several readability metrics exist that can be used to quantify the readability of English-language text, such as Flesch–Kincaid readability test, Coleman–Liau index, and automated readability index [45]. In this study, we evaluated readability by using graded vocabulary lists in a Chinese proficiency test, namely Hanyu Shuiping Kaoshi [HSK], and by using English-language readability measures. The total number of words in the HSK lists is 23,760, which are divided into six levels.

Readability R corresponded to the combination of the average number of words per sentence and the average HSK grade per word [57] and was calculated as follows:

$$ \boldsymbol{R} = \mathbf{ln} \biggl( \frac{\boldsymbol{W}}{\boldsymbol{N}} \biggr) + \frac{\sum_{\boldsymbol{i} = \mathbf{1}}^{\boldsymbol{T}} \boldsymbol{Li}}{\boldsymbol{T}}, $$
(3)

where W represents the word count of a given answer, N represents the total number of sentences, T represents the total number of words assigned an HSK grade, and \(L_{i}\) represents the score of the ith word assigned an HSK grade (\(i \in [1, T]\)). We scored words from HSK levels 1 to 6 as 1 to 6, respectively (\(L_{i}\in [1,6]\)). Readability is an inverse measure; the higher the readability score of a text, the higher the complexity of that text. We further provided an example to illustrate how to calculate readability in the Additional file 1.

We manually coded the questions into experience-based, opinion-based, and fact-based queries. Experience-based questions require the provision of information on the personal experience of respondents (e.g., “How is your experience with using Google Pay?”). Opinion-based questions inquire about respondents’ opinions (e.g., “Why is Lewis Hamilton so generally disliked?). Fact-based questions inquire about objective facts (e.g., “Is it possible to fly a rocket through Jupiter?”).

3.3 Data analysis

We performed hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) by estimating the maximum likelihood across two levels (i.e., within-question knowledge contribution behavior and between-question differences in knowledge contribution). The within-question features (i.e., marketplace of ideas, participation features, discourse features, and temporal features) were postulated to be nested within the between-question factors (i.e., question types and number of answers) in influencing the dynamics of information accumulation. HLM is a useful method for analyzing longitudinal data, enabling researchers studying the dynamics of information accumulation over time to fit various advanced regression models into longitudinal datasets.

A lower-level (i.e., level 1) model presented the information accumulation for each question as a linear function of the marketplace of ideas, participation feature, temporal features, and discourse features. The parameters of the lower-level variables were considered dependent variables in the higher-level (i.e., level 2) model. Level 2 parameters were predicted using the question type and number of answers. To estimate the goodness of fit, we established a null model: a model with no level-1 or level-2 predictors. We then compared the reduction of the log-likelihood ratio (i.e., −2LL) of the null model with that of the full model (Table 1).

Table 1 HLM regression for information accumulation

4 Analytical results

4.1 Dynamics of information accumulation among questions

For all 1832 questions, the number of answers exhibited a typical skewed distribution, indicating that most questions received few answers, and that few questions received many answers. The number of answers for the 1,832 questions ranged from 10 to 2114. For 75% of the questions, the number of answers was <40. Figure 3(a) displays the truncated distribution of questions that had fewer than 100 answers.

Figure 3
figure 3

Distribution of the number of answers; (a) all answers; (b) truncated distribution of questions that had fewer than 100 answers

To represent the dynamics of information accumulation, we selected questions that had a typical number of answers. As shown in Fig. 4(a), we ranked the frequency of answers to all questions and selected questions receiving 10 to 21 answers (corresponding to the tenth rank of the number of answers). The dynamic of information accumulation with questions receiving less than 80 answers is shown in Fig. 4(b). In Fig. 4(a), the x-axis represents the order of answers (i.e., the nth answer), and the y-axis represents the mean value of information accumulation between the nth answer and the first \((n - 1)\)th answer. The colors of the lines represent questions with different numbers of answers. For example, the blue line represents the dynamics of information accumulation for all questions with 10 answers. The shadow represents the 95% confidence interval.

Figure 4
figure 4

Dynamics of information accumulation. (a) questions receiving 10 to 21 answers; (b) questions receiving less than 80 answers

All questions with different numbers of answers exhibited a similar pattern of information accumulation. The information accumulation of the (\(n + 1\))th answer to the preceding answer (i.e., the nth answer) decreased rapidly during the early stage of knowledge construction; that is, the increase in informativeness tapered off rapidly over this period. With the completion of knowledge construction, the information accumulation of the (\(n + 1\))th answer to the nth answer leveled off; no further information was accumulated at the end of the answer sequence. This finding is consistent with that of Keegan and Tan [30], who reported that subsequent answers provided on Wikipedia to COVID-19-related questions exhibited a consistent decrease in information novelty. This pattern indicated that the online knowledge construction process is completed through ongoing changes rather than sudden shifts.

4.2 Factors affecting information accumulation

Table 1 lists the HLM results, with the features of questions (level 2) presented as aggregated variables and the characteristics of answers (level 1) presented as individual variables. To determine how well the model fit the data, a null model (i.e., model 1; that is, a model with no level-1 or level-2 predictors) was first estimated. Model 2 was a full model, also known as an intercept-as-outcome model. Model fit in HLM was determined using the log-likelihood ratio, a deviance statistic. A large log-likelihood ratio indicates an unfavorable model fit, and a small log-likelihood ratio indicates a favorable model fit. The model fit can be determined by comparing the deviances between the full and null models. The difference between the two deviance statistics follows a chi-square distribution, in which the degree of freedom equals the difference in the number of parameters estimated between the two models. If the \(\chi ^{2}\) value is statistically significant, the model with a low deviance value has a significantly more favorable fit. The present model explained 47% of the variance, which is significantly more than that in a previous study investigating the individual information contribution of platform users [58].

Both within-question–level and between-question–level variables in the model significantly explained information accumulation (Table 1). The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of the model was 0.18, indicating that approximately 18% of the variance in information accumulation could be explained by between-question differences. This ICC was greater than zero, suggesting that HLM regression was required to determine the fixed effects, as opposed to conventional methods such as analysis of variance [59].

Table 1 displays the HLM regression results. The effects of Gini coefficients of both the answer length (\(\beta = -\)0.11; \(p < 0.001\)) and the number of comments (\(\beta = -\)0.11; \(p < 0.001\)) in information accumulation were significantly negative. This indicates that the more unequal is the distribution of users’ viewpoints to a question, the lower the likelihood of information accumulation over time. For a given question, a more competitive marketplace of ideas contributes more to information accumulation than a more monopolistic marketplace of ideas does. Therefore, Hypothesis 1 was supported.

Highly active users (i.e., answering frequencies ranked in the top 1%) and active users (i.e., answering frequencies ranked in the top 2%–10%) contributed significantly less to information accumulation than did ordinary users (i.e., answering frequencies ranked in the bottom 90%). Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was not supported.

The time interval between answers at the two preceding time points (i.e., time t and time \(t-1\)) negatively affected the informativeness of answers at the subsequent time point (\(\beta = -\)0.01; \(p < 0.001\)). The longer the time interval between two answers was, the lower the informativeness of answers at the subsequent time point was. Therefore, Hypothesis 3 was supported.

The order of answers significantly predicted the dynamics of information accumulation (\(\beta = -1.44\); \(p< 0.001\)), indicating that the knowledge construction process is a function of the answer sequence. Therefore, Hypothesis 4 was supported.

The sentiment of the online discussion exerted no effect on information accumulation. Therefore, Hypothesis 5 was not supported. This result is in line with that of Harper et al. [60], who observed that on Q&A sites, expressions of gratitude did not significantly predict answers to a question.

Answers with a higher readability score reduced the informativeness of subsequent answers (\(\beta = -0.01\); \(p < 0.01\)). Because the readability value of text represents its complexity, the results indicate that the complexity level of the online discussion reduced the accumulation of information. Therefore, Hypothesis 6 was supported.

Regarding control variables, the number of answers was determined to negatively influence information accumulation (\(\beta = -0.004\); \(p < 0.05\)). Information accumulation decreased with an increase in the number of answers (see Additional file 1 for the follow-up test of interaction effects between the number of answers and Gini coefficient). Regarding the question type, compared with experience-based questions, threads for fact-based questions (\(\beta = -0.04\); \(p < 0.001\)) exhibited a lower level of information accumulation.

5 Discussion

In this study, online knowledge construction was facilitated in a competitive marketplace of ideas by information accumulation. By contrast, the monopolization of the marketplace restricted the community’s ability to progress and innovate in terms of thoughts and ideas. Participation features, temporal features, and discourse features also affected the dynamics of information accumulation. The results demonstrated that Q&A sites are not only a large repository for knowledge but also a new field for information exchange and knowledge construction. The emergence of Q&A sites has transformed knowledge construction into a collaborative and thoroughly socialized process [61].

Most of the questions exhibited a similar pattern of information accumulation. New information accumulation in follow-up answers initially increased rapidly but then decreased gradually. The informativeness of answers decreased rapidly during the early stage of knowledge construction, and no additional information was accumulated at the end of the answer sequence. Temporal features, namely the interevent time and the order of answers, also affected the dynamics of information accumulation. A short between-answer interval increased information accumulation. This suggests that knowledge construction on Q&A sites is a dynamic process through which new information is continually accumulated.

The model factors indicate that online knowledge construction involves continual collaboration between users. Highly active users did not contribute substantially to information accumulation. Highly active users may be highly conforming and may demonstrate homogeneity in their language use on the platform. Therefore, knowledge construction on Q&A sites is not dependent on the contributions of such users despite their high number of contributions on the platforms. Instead, the knowledge construction process is characterized by limited contributions from a large number of less active users [62]. This result is consistent with the Ortega hypothesis, which states that a large pool of ordinary contributors is essential to a system’s overall functioning.

Our results indicate that the characteristics of the marketplace of ideas are major factors that drive information accumulation on Q&A sites. Knowledge is more likely to accumulate in a competitive marketplace of ideas than in a monopolistic marketplace of ideas. This conclusion is based on the observation that equal distributions of answer lengths and numbers of comments promoted information accumulation.

5.1 Possible explanations for the facilitation of information accumulation by a competitive marketplace of ideas

We present a possible explanation for the facilitation of information accumulation in a competitive marketplace of ideas. Because a competitive marketplace of ideas lacks dominant viewpoints [2], it is characterized by disagreement and dissonance. The development of knowledge is triggered by individual cognitive disturbance, that is, the prolific resolution of socio-cognitive conflict [62]. Cognitive disturbances result from individuals’ realization that others’ cognitive schemata differ from theirs, as represented by disagreement and dissonance on the Q&A sites. Addressing the disturbances in a productive manner facilitates learning and collaborative knowledge construction. In the long term, this leads to innovation and information accumulation [62, 63].

5.2 Perspective shift in research on knowledge construction

Theories and models have indicated that collaborative knowledge construction occurs at both intrapersonal and interpersonal levels. At the intrapersonal level, psychological and cognitive systems contribute to users’ knowledge construction and retrieval behaviors. These behaviors include the retrieval of knowledge from long-term memory [64], the elaboration of knowledge [65], and the externalization and internalization of knowledge. Knowledge contribution occurs through the internal processes of assimilation and accommodation. On Q&A sites, users refine and reassemble their knowledge within a certain domain. Personal knowledge must be logically and comprehensibly conveyed as an answer. This requires the in-depth processing and clarification of information [62]. Accordingly, knowledge construction is considered to be a product of users’ active participation in an online environment [66].

At the interpersonal level, knowledge contributors who utilize knowledge resources in the form of electronic artifacts constitute a community of individuals who share a common interest and are willing to share their ideas with other community members or to respond to their ideas. Peer facilitation techniques that increase knowledge contributions have been studied in the literature. Peer facilitation refers to online discussion facilitated by participants in knowledge contribution settings [67]. Peer facilitators have been demonstrated to be instrumental in shaping the discourse, and the use of peer facilitation results in effective online discussions [68, 69].

In sum, numerous studies have framed knowledge construction as either an individual-level or bilateral behavior. These framings do not holistically reflect the dynamic pattern of knowledge construction, and knowledge is erroneously considered to exist on its own rather than being a dynamic product on which users’ interactive sequences are based. This misperception can be attributed to the development of models using small data samples of Q&A sites, which cannot accurately model and theorize the dynamics of knowledge construction.

This study contributes to the literature on knowledge construction by shifting the focus from intrapersonal and interpersonal communication to the process of knowledge construction at the question–answer level. The sequence perspective emphasizes that no hidden correct knowledge awaits discovery. Knowledge is constructed when answers are crafted and circulated through an online community. The manner in which knowledge develops on Q&A sites indicates that knowledge is rarely purely individual and is never static [62, 70]. Rather, knowledge construction on Q&A sites is cumulative and depends on previous answers.

5.3 Practical implications

The results correspond to a nonessentialist view of knowledge construction and contain practical implications for the management of Q&A sites. The contributions of less active platform users have been considerably underestimated. For example, Amazon mainly reward their top knowledge contributors. Q&A sites should reward users on the basis of the informativeness of their answers rather than on the basis of their answering frequency.

6 Limitations

This study had several limitations. First, this study explored the dynamics of information accumulation in the context of a Q&A site. Collaborative knowledge construction varies subtly among different online knowledge sharing platforms. For example, Wikipedia is an open-source repository of reference knowledge to which users contribute knowledge by collaboratively writing and editing articles [12]. The premise that different online knowledge-sharing platforms share elements of knowledge construction is reasonable. Future research could generalize the results to other online knowledge sharing platforms.

In addition, this study focused on a Chinese Q&A site. The culture of cooperation may play crucial roles in information accumulation. Therefore, the study results may not be generalizable to sites in different languages or sites aimed at users from different cultures. Whether the effects of the marketplace of ideas on knowledge construction are generalizable to online knowledge-sharing platforms in other languages and countries warrants further research.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Notes

  1. Sina Technology. (2018, December 13). Live|Zhuhu announces 220 million users, doubles growth in one year. https://tech.sina.com.cn/i/2018-12-13/doc-ihmutuec8882383.shtml

Abbreviations

Q&A sites:

Question-and-answer Websites

OKSPs:

Online Knowledge Sharing Platforms

HSK:

the graded vocabulary for Chinese Proficiency Test (HSK)

HLM:

Hierarchical Linear Modeling

ICC:

Intraclass Correlation Coefficient

References

  1. Lerner A (1995) The concept of monopoly and the measurement of monopoly power. In: Estrin S, Marin A (eds) Essential readings in economics. Macmillan Education UK, London, pp 55–76

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  2. Gunawardena CN, Lowe CA, Anderson T (1997) Analysis of a global online debate and the development of an interaction analysis model for examining social construction of knowledge in computer conferencing. J Educ Comput Res 17:397–431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Cole JR, Cole S (1972) The Ortega hypothesis: citation analysis suggests that only a few scientists contribute to scientific progress. Science 178:368–375. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.178.4059.368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lachenmayer D (2019) Let it flow: the monopolization of academic content providers and how it threatens the democratization of information. Ser Libr 75:70–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361526X.2018.1556189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Tatum C, LaFrance M (2009) Wikipedia as a knowledge production laboratory: the case of neoliberalism. e-research: transformation in scholarly practice. Routledge, New York, pp 310–327.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Hewitt J (2005) Toward an understanding of how threads die in asynchronous computer conferences. J Learn Sci 14:567–589. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327809jls1404_4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Chhabra A, Iyengar SRS (2020) Who writes Wikipedia?: an investigation from the perspective of Ortega and Newton hypotheses. In: Proceedings of the 16th international symposium on open collaboration. ACM, virtual conference Spain, pp 1–11

    Google Scholar 

  8. Bereiter C, Scardamalia M (2003) Learning to work creatively with knowledge. Powerful learning environments: Unravelling basic components and dimensions 55–68

  9. Scardamalia M, Bereiter C (1994) Computer support for knowledge-building communities. J Learn Sci 3:265–283

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Scardamalia M, Bereiter C (1999) Schools as knowledge-building organizations. Today’s children, tomorrow’s society: The developmental health and wealth of nations, 274–289

  11. Latour B, Woolgar S (1986) Laboratory life: the construction of scientific facts. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  12. Matei SA, Abu Jabal A, Bertino E (2018) Social-collaborative determinants of content quality in online knowledge production systems: comparing Wikipedia and stack overflow. Soc Netw Anal Min 8:36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13278-018-0512-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Maggiore P (2011) Viewer discretion is advised: disconnects between the marketplace of ideas and social media used to communicate information during emergencies and public health crises. Mich Telecomm & Tech L Rev 18:627.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Bennett A, Seyis D (2021) The online market’s invisible hand: internet media and rising populism. Political Studies 003232172110332. https://doi.org/10.1177/00323217211033230

  15. Siebert FS (1956) Four theories of the press: the authoritarian, libertarian, social responsibility, and Soviet communist concepts of what the press should be and do. University of Illinois Press, Urbana

    Google Scholar 

  16. Manohar S (2006) Look who’s talking now: “Choose Life” license plates and deceptive government speech. Yale Law Policy Rev 25:229–237

    Google Scholar 

  17. Posner RA (1977) Monopoly in the marketplace of ideas. Yale Law J 86:567. https://doi.org/10.2307/795802

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Piaget J (1970) Science of education and the psychology of the child. Trans D Coltman

  19. De A, Valera I, Ganguly N, Bhattacharya S, Rodriguez MG (2016) Learning and forecasting opinion dynamics in social networks. arXiv:1506.05474 [physics]

  20. Capineri C, Haklay M, Huang H, Antoniou V, Kettunen J, Ostermann F, Purves R (2016) European handbook of crowdsourced geographic information. Ubiquity Press, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  21. Stewart O, Lubensky D, Huerta JM (2010) Crowdsourcing participation inequality: a SCOUT model for the enterprise domain. In: Proceedings of the ACM SIGKDD workshop on human computation - HCOMP’10. ACM Press, Washington, p 30

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  22. Kittur A, Kraut RE (2010) Beyond Wikipedia: coordination and conflict in online production groups. In: Proceedings of the 2010 ACM conference on computer supported cooperative work - CSCW’10. ACM Press, Savannah, p 215

    Google Scholar 

  23. Muchnik L, Pei S, Parra LC, Reis SDS, Andrade Jr JS, Havlin S, Makse HA (2013) Origins of power-law degree distribution in the heterogeneity of human activity in social networks. Sci Rep 3:1783. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep01783

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Serrano A, Arroyo J, Hassan S (2018) Participation inequality in wikis: a temporal analysis using wikichron. In: Proceedings of the 14th international symposium on open collaboration, pp 1–7

    Google Scholar 

  25. Movshovitz-Attias D, Movshovitz-Attias Y, Steenkiste P, Faloutsos C (2013) Analysis of the reputation system and user contributions on a question answering website: StackOverflow. In: 2013 IEEE/ACM international conference on advances in social networks analysis and mining (ASONAM 2013), pp 886–893

    Google Scholar 

  26. Peng T-Q, Zhu JJ (2020) Mobile phone use as sequential processes: from discrete behaviors to sessions of behaviors and trajectories of sessions. J Comput-Mediat Commun 25:129–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Krampen G (2010) Acceleration of citing behavior after the millennium? Exemplary bibliometric reference analyses for psychology journals. Scientometrics 83:507–513. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-009-0093-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Kushner T, Sharma A (2020) Bursts of activity: temporal patterns of help-seeking and support in online mental health forums. In: Proceedings of the web conference 2020. ACM, Taipei Taiwan, pp 2906–2912

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  29. Vasilescu B, Serebrenik A, Devanbu P, Filkov V (2014) How social Q&A sites are changing knowledge sharing in open source software communities. In: Proceedings of the 17th ACM conference on computer supported cooperative work & social computing, pp 342–354

    Google Scholar 

  30. Keegan BC, Tan C (2020) A quantitative portrait of Wikipedia’s high-tempo collaborations during the 2020 coronavirus pandemic. arXiv:2006.08899 [physics]

  31. Barrett FJ (1998) Coda—creativity and improvisation in jazz and organizations: implications for organizational learning. Organ Sci 9:605–622. https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.9.5.605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Weick KE, Roberts KH (1993) Collective mind in organizations: heedful interrelating on flight decks. Adm Sci Q 38:357. https://doi.org/10.2307/2393372

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Faraj S, Sproull L (2000) Coordinating expertise in software development teams. Manag Sci 46:1554–1568. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.46.12.1554.12072

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Yates D, Wagner C, Majchrzak A (2009) Factors affecting shapers of organizational wikis. J Am Soc Inf Sci. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.21266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Anderson A, Huttenlocher D, Kleinberg J, Leskovec J (2012) Discovering value from community activity on focused question answering sites: a case study of stack overflow. In: Proceedings of the 18th ACM SIGKDD international conference on knowledge discovery and data mining, pp 850–858

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  36. Xu W (Wayne) Zhang C (2018) Sentiment, richness, authority, and relevance model of information sharing during social crises—the case of #MH370 tweets. Comput Hum Behav 89:199–206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.07.041

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Novielli N, Calefato F, Lanubile F (2014) Towards discovering the role of emotions in stack overflow. In: Proceedings of the 6th international workshop on social software engineering. ACM, Hong, Kong China, pp 33–36

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  38. Grigore M, Rosenkranz C (2011) Increasing the willingness to collaborate online: An analysis of sentiment-driven interactions in peer content production. In: ICIS 2011 Proceedings

    Google Scholar 

  39. Jiao Y, Li C, Wu F, Mei Q (2018) Find the conversation killers: a predictive study of thread-ending posts. In: Proceedings of the 2018 world wide web conference on world wide web - WWW’18. ACM Press, Lyon, pp 1145–1154

    Google Scholar 

  40. Wang L, Cardie C (2016) A piece of my mind: A sentiment analysis approach for online dispute detection. https://doi.org/10.48550/ARXIV.1606.05704

  41. Hegselmann R (2002) Opinion dynamics and bounded confidence models, analysis, and simulation. J Artif Soc Soc Simul 5:3

    Google Scholar 

  42. Krapp A (1999) Interest, motivation and learning: an educational-psychological perspective. Eur J Psychol Educ 14:23–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Colombetti G (2005) Appraising valence. J Conscious Stud 12:103–126

    Google Scholar 

  44. Dale E, Chall JS (1949) The concept of readability. Elem Engl 26:19–26

    Google Scholar 

  45. Setia S, Iyengar SRS, Verma AA, Dubey N (2021) Is Wikipedia easy to understand?: a study beyond conventional readability metrics. In: Wojtkiewicz K, Treur J, Pimenidis E, Maleszka M (eds) Advances in computational collective intelligence. Springer, Cham, pp 175–187

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  46. Nwogu KN (1991) Structure of science popularizations: a genre-analysis approach to the schema of popularized medical texts. https://doi.org/10.1016/0889-4906(91)90004-G. English for Specific Purposes 10:111–123

  47. Zhang Y, Lu T, Phang C, Zhang C (2019) Scientific knowledge communication in online Q&A communities: linguistic devices as a tool to increase the popularity and perceived professionalism of knowledge contribution. J Assoc Inf Syst 20(3). https://doi.org/10.17705/1jais.00

  48. Calsamiglia H, Ferrero CL (2003) Role and position of scientific voices: reported speech in the media. Discourse Stud. 5:147–173. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445603005002308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Harper FM, Weinberg J, Logie J, Konstan JA (2010) Question types in social Q&A sites. https://doi.org/10.5210/fm.v15i7.2913. FM

  50. Kim S, Oh JS, Oh S (2008) Best-answer selection criteria in a social Q&A site from the user-oriented relevance perspective. Proc Am Soc Info Sci Tech 44:1–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/meet.1450440256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Orben A, Przybylski AK (2019) The association between adolescent well-being and digital technology use. Nat Hum Behav 3:173–182. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Qi P, Zhang Y, Manning CD (2020) Stay hungry, stay focused: generating informative and specific questions in information-seeking conversations. arXiv:2004.14530 [cs]

  53. Le Q, Mikolov T (2014) Distributed representations of sentences and documents. In: Proceedings of the 31st international conference on machine learning. PMLR, pp 1188–1196

    Google Scholar 

  54. Mandal A, Chaki R, Saha S, Ghosh K, Pal A, Ghosh S (2017) Measuring similarity among legal court case documents. In: Proceedings of the 10th annual ACM India compute conference on ZZZ - Compute’17. ACM Press, Bhopal, pp 1–9

    Google Scholar 

  55. Zheng Y, Cui Z, Xu Y, Li H, Jiang Z (2020) Generating adversarial examples for sentiment classifier of Chinese sentences. In: 2020 6th international symposium on system and software reliability (ISSSR). IEEE, Chengdu, pp 27–32

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  56. Geng S, Niu B, Feng Y, Huang M (2020) Understanding the focal points and sentiment of learners in MOOC reviews: a machine learning and SC-LIWC-based approach. Br J Educ Technol 51:1785–1803. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjet.12999

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Yang S (1971) A readability formula for Chinese language. Dissertation, University of Wisconsin

  58. Zhang L, Han Y, Zhou J-L, Liu Y-S, Wu Y (2020) Influence of intrinsic motivations on the continuity of scientific knowledge contribution to online knowledge-sharing platforms. Public Underst Sci 096366252097078. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662520970782

  59. Kreft IG, De Leeuw J, Aiken LS (1995) The effect of different forms of centering in hierarchical linear models. Multivar Behav Res 30:1–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Harper FM, Raban D, Rafaeli S, Konstan JA (2008) Predictors of answer quality in online Q&A sites. In: Proceeding of the twenty-sixth annual CHI conference on human factors in computing systems - CHI’08. ACM Press, Florence, p 865

    Google Scholar 

  61. Lievrouw LA (2010) Social media and the production of knowledge: a return to little science? Soc Epistemol 24:219–237. https://doi.org/10.1080/02691728.2010.499177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Holtz P, Kimmerle J, Cress U (2018) Using big data techniques for measuring productive friction in mass collaboration online environments. Comput-Support Collab Learn 13:439–456. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-018-9285-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Hagel J 3rd, Brown JS (2005) Productive friction: how difficult business partnerships can accelerate innovation. Harv Bus Rev 83(82–91):148

    Google Scholar 

  64. Baddeley A, Logie R, Bressi S, Sala SD, Spinnler H (1986) Dementia and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol, A Hum Exp Psychol 38:603–618. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748608401616

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Craik FIM, Lockhart RS (1972) Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 11:671–684. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Leseman PPM (2000) Bilingual vocabulary development of Turkish preschoolers in the Netherlands. J Multiling Multicult Dev 21:93–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434630008666396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Ashwin P (2003) Peer facilitation and how it contributes to the development of a more social view of learning. Research in Post-Compulsory Education 8:005. https://doi.org/10.1080/13596740300200137

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  68. Rourke L, Anderson T (2002) Using peer teams to lead online discussions. JIME 2002:1. https://doi.org/10.5334/2002-1

  69. Gilbert PK, Dabbagh N (2005) How to structure online discussions for meaningful discourse: a case study. Br J Educ Technol 36:5–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8535.2005.00434.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Oeberst A, Kimmerle J, Cress U (2016) What is knowledge? Who creates it? Who possesses it? The need for novel answers to old questions. In: Cress U, Moskaliuk J, Jeong H (eds) Mass collaboration and education. Springer, Cham, pp 105–124

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We highly appreciated the coding of data done by Yilin Deng and Jinzhuo Liu from Beijing Normal University. The manuscript has been submitted solely to EPJ Data Science and that it has not been previously published, either in whole or in part, nor have the findings been posted online. All authors listed in the byline have agreed to the byline order and to submission of the manuscript in this form.

Authors’ information

Lun Zhang is currently an associate professor at Beijing Normal University. She is a Tang Scholar supported by the Cyrus Tang Foundation. Her current research projects focus on knowledge sharing on social media sites. Her research outputs appeared in Information Processing & Management, Computers in Human Behavior, Internet Research and other leading journals in the field of information science and communication. Yong-Ning Li is currently studying at the School of Systems Science, Beijing Normal University. She is a PhD candidate, and her research field is computational social sciences. Tai-Quan Peng is an Associate Professor at the Department of Communication, Michigan State University. His current research focuses on computational social science, health communication, political communication, and mobile analytics. Ye Wu is currently a professor at the School of Journalism and Communication, Beijing Normal University. His research interests include computational communication and social networks. His research outputs appeared in Science, Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, and Public Understanding of Sciences.

Funding

This publication has received the support of (China) National Planning Office of Philosophy and Social Science (20BXW102).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Lun Zhang conceived the research project, interpreted the results, wrote the article. Yong-Ning Li performed technical work, interpreted the results, helped write the article. Tai-Quan Peng conceived the research project, interpreted the results. Ye Wu helped with the technical work, interpreted the results. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ye Wu.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary information (DOCX 335 kB)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, L., Li, YN., Peng, TQ. et al. Dynamics of the social construction of knowledge: an empirical study of Zhihu in China. EPJ Data Sci. 11, 35 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1140/epjds/s13688-022-00346-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1140/epjds/s13688-022-00346-6

Keywords